While many factors are at play, we can blame our brains—at least to some degree—for our poor saving habits, according to a new study.
The average American working-age couple has saved only $5,000 for retirement, while 43 percent of working-age families have no retirement savings at all, according to a 2016 analysis of a Federal Reserve survey. And as of 2017, people were saving less than 3 percent of their personal disposable income, a figure that has long been in a downward spiral, according to the US Bureau of Economic Analysis.
“Fundamentally it comes down to this: saving is less valuable to our brains…”
Humans have a cognitive bias toward earning, which makes us unconsciously spend more brain power on earning than on saving, according to the study. Moment to moment and day by day, our brains are less attentive to, and may devalue, saving. Over time, that could affect our future wealth. And the cognitive bias is so powerful that it can even warp our sense of time, the researchers show.
“Fundamentally it comes down to this: saving is less valuable to our brains, which devote less attentional resources to it,” says study coauthor Adam Anderson, associate professor of human development at Cornell University. “It’s more than a financial problem of making ends meet. Our brains find saving more difficult to attend to.”
Color-coded opportunities
In the study, the researchers created their own experimental micro-economy in which individuals could earn or save money by responding to how different colors signified these opportunities. They also gave study participants a timing perception task with these same colors, measuring how quickly they processed colors as an implicit index of the potency of earning and saving for the brain.
In the economic task, participants could earn or save money based on how quickly and accurately they performed an activity involving color circles. In the temporal perception task, they indicated which of the color circles appeared first, when the circles were presented side-by-side with different delays between them.
“Even without bills to pay, our brains put a thumb on the scales, making it easier for us to earn than save…”
In the first experiment, 87.5 percent of the participants earned more than they saved. And 75 percent developed warped temporal perceptions of the colors: They reported seeing earning colors appear on the computer screen first when, in fact, the savings colors did. In subsequent experiments, this temporal bias occurred even when color associations with earning or saving were hidden and likely unconscious.
The researchers have termed this bias “savings posteriority.” Savings is treated as a later concern, biased in each moment by the brain to have less temporal priority. While we must earn before we can save, our brains may blind us to opportunities to save, in a way that fundamentally distorts our perceptions, they says.
“Even without bills to pay, our brains put a thumb on the scales, making it easier for us to earn than save,” Anderson says. That’s because the brain may be fundamentally inattentive to saving relative to earning. “Saving is so devalued and unattended that we perceive events associated with saving as occurring later in time,” says coauthor Eve De Rosa, an associate professor of human development.
The warped time perception may or may not be a mechanism for the cognitive bias to earn more than save, Anderson says. “At a minimum, it’s an indication of how strong this bias is, that it can even warp our perception of time,” he says. “Imagine what it could do to our bank accounts.”Depression and anxiety can cost you retirement savings
Even when the researchers changed the economic task to ensure study participants received an equal amount of earnings and savings, the temporal bias persisted. And the bias against saving occurred whether researchers defined saving as preventing the loss of what the participants already earned or as putting away money for future use. Either way, the results were the same: Earning beat saving.
How to fix this tendency
The researchers point out that the bias is likely unconsciously learned, not necessarily one that has been handed down through evolution. That’s good news, De Rosa says: “If you’ve learned it, you can unlearn it.”
“Earning and saving may involve flexing different muscles; the more we pay attention to saving opportunities, the more we exercise that mental muscle,” says Anderson. Those who want to save more could start by trying attentional retraining—that is, practice paying attention to saving. The benefit is not so much in the everyday cash value of what one saves; it’s in building the brain’s capacity to pay attention to saving, which, like money in the bank, will increase over time.
“It’s practicing attention and intention to save, to strengthen the value of it for your brain. It’s not the amount of dollars that matters,” Anderson says.
“And you’ll probably see other avenues and opportunities as your brain learns to value saving,” De Rosa adds.
The paper appears in Nature Communications.
Source: Cornell University
Related Books
at InnerSelf Market and Amazon